Navigation

 ·   Wiki Home
 ·   Wiki Help
 ·   Categories
 ·   Title List
 ·   Uncategorized Pages
 ·   Random Page
 ·   Recent Changes
 ·   RSS
 ·   Atom
 ·   What Links Here

Active Members:

Search:

 

Create or Find Page:

 

View Spurious Submission Hoaxes

Type: Literary Hoax.
Summary: A hoaxer takes a well-regarded work and alters a few key details, such as the title and name of the author. He then submits the manuscript to a publisher. Usually the publisher fails to recognize the work and rejects it.


Every year publishers receive thousands of unsolicited manuscripts. Of these, perhaps a handful get published. Why is this? Publishers might attribute it to the rarity of finding in the slush pile anything worthy of being published. Rejected authors, however, have long argued otherwise. They suspect that publishers are either a) not reading unsolicited manuscripts, despite claiming to do so, or b) are incompetent to recognize talent when they see it.

The spurious submission hoax plays to these suspicions. In this hoax, a hoaxer sends a publisher a manuscript that is highly worthy of being published since, typically, it has already been published to much acclaim. However, the hoaxer conceals the true authorship of the manuscript, presenting it as a new submission by an unknown author. In most cases, the publisher fails to recognize the manuscript for what it is and rejects it. The hoaxer then offers this rejection as evidence of the flawed nature of the publishing industry’s selection process.

The earliest known example of this hoax dates back to the late nineteenth century, when a would-be author, frustrated by frequent rejections, sent around a re-titled copy of John Milton’s Samson Agonistes to publishers and magazine editors, none of whom recognized it.  In recent years, instances of spurious-submission hoaxes have been occurring with increasing frequency.

Neglected Literature

One issue raised by the spurious submission hoax is the problem of neglected literature. The literary community likes to imagine itself as a meritocracy, in which works of merit eventually receive recognition. However, the consistent success of spurious submission hoaxes challenges this rosy view of things.

If publishers cannot recognize the quality of an acknowledged masterpiece, can they be expected to recognize the worth of an unknown work in the slush pile? And even if a work does get published, what if it never finds a critic to champion it and therefore languishes in obscurity?

Critics of the publishing industry have long argued that the small collection of works that have made it into press and received critical acclaim may owe their success not to their inherent brilliance, but rather to luck. The works happened to be in the right place, at the right time, and caught someone’s eye. There may be thousands of worthy, but neglected, works of literature out there, critics suggest, sitting unread on shelves or filed away in desk drawers, their authors not having been lucky enough to win in the literary lottery.

The Matthew Effect

Critics of the publishing industry also argue that far too much attention is paid to the fame or celebrity of the author, rather than the quality of the work. Well-known authors get published easily, even if their later works decline in quality, and bestseller lists are regularly filled with works by celebrity authors who possess no discernible literary talent.

In essence, the publishing industry heaps rewards upon those who are already famous, leaving talented authors who happen to be unknown to struggle in obscurity. As the popular saying goes, “Nothing succeeds like success.” This phenomenon is known as the “Matthew Effect,” a term coined by the sociologist Robert Merton. The term derives from a line in the Bible’s Book of Matthew:

“For unto every one that hath shall be given, and he shall have abundance: but from him that hath not shall be taken away even that which he hath.” (Matthew XXV:29, KJV).

The Psychology of Evaluation

The spurious submission hoax also offers insights into the psychology of evaluation. Psychologists suggest that we constantly use “schemas” to evaluate the world around us. Schemas are like mental shortcuts, assumptions we make about the qualities of people and things in the world around us. We tend to interpret the world in such a way that it corresponds with our preconceived schemas.

The researcher Harold Kelley illustrated how schemas work in a classic experiment conducted in 1950. He arranged for a guest lecturer to visit his class. Before the class, he told some of his students (randomly selected) that the guest lecturer was “a very warm person.” But he told other students that the guest lecturer was “a rather cold person.” After the class, he asked the students to evaluate the lecturer. Those who had been told the guest lecturer was rather cold gave him much lower ratings than those who had been told he was warm. In addition, the students who had been told the lecturer was warm found him much more humorous than those who had not been told this. The students were using the schema supplied to them earlier to interpret the lecturer’s behavior. Their expectations influenced their perception of the man.

Schemas help to explain why readers at publishing houses might fail to see the quality of an unsolicited manuscript. The readers might assume that unsolicited manuscripts are of lesser quality, therefore they read the submissions with a more critical eye (if they bother to closely read them at all). Their expectation that unsolicited manuscripts are less worthy of consideration then becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.

In other words, a reader’s evaluation of a manuscript will depend on the context in which they receive it. They might react to the same work very differently depending on whether it is given to them by an influential agent who showers it with praise, or if they find it in the unsolicited slush pile.

Examples of Spurious Submission Hoaxes

  • Milton Rejected (late 19th century): An author submitted copies of John Milton’s Samson Agonistes to publishers and magazine editors.
  • The Steps Experiment (1975): Chuck Ross submitted Jerzy Kosinski’s Steps to fourteen publishers and twenty-six literary agents, all of whom rejected it.
  • The Peters and Ceci Experiment (1980): Inspired by Ross’s Steps experiment, Douglas Peters and Stephen Ceci took ten published scientific articles, changed the name of the author and title of the article, and resubmitted them to the same journal in which they had previously been published. The majority of editors rejected the articles without recognizing they had already been published in their own journal.
  • Casablanca Rejected (1982): Chuck Ross submitted the script of Casablanca to over 200 movie agents, many of whom rejected it.
  • Doris Lessing as Jane Somers (1982): Lessing used a pseudonym to submit a new book by herself to publishers, to test how easily she could get published as an unknown author.
  • Claire Chazal Experiment (2000): A French magazine mocked a celebrity author by submitting a disguised copy of her novel to her own publisher, which rejected it.
  • VS Naipaul Rejected (2004)
  • Patrick White Rejected
  • Jane Austen Rejected (2007)

References

  • Kelley, H.H. (1950). “The warm-cold variable in first impressions of persons.” Journal of Personality. 18:431-439.
  • Tabachnick, Stephen E. (1981). “The Problem of Neglected Literature.” College English. 43(1): 32-44.

Categories: